Published at : 28 Jul 2023
Volume : IJtech
Vol 14, No 5 (2023)
DOI : https://doi.org/10.14716/ijtech.v14i5.4856
Muhammad Lutfi Shadiq Zainy | Faculty of Industrial Technology, Institut Teknologi Bandung, JL. Ganesha No. 10, Bandung 40132, Indonesia |
Gradiyan Budi Pratama | Faculty of Industrial Technology, Institut Teknologi Bandung, JL. Ganesha No. 10, Bandung 40132, Indonesia |
Rifko Rahmat Kurnianto | Faculty of Industrial Technology, Institut Teknologi Bandung, JL. Ganesha No. 10, Bandung 40132, Indonesia |
Hardianto Iridiastadi | Faculty of Industrial Technology, Institut Teknologi Bandung, JL. Ganesha No. 10, Bandung 40132, Indonesia |
Fatigue from driving is a risk factor in many road
traffic accidents and the association between task duration and fatigue, in
particular, has been a subject of research interest. Fatigue (and sleepiness) seems to plague many
Indonesian commercial drivers whose jobs involve long-duration driving, but
this issue is still rarely quantified and investigated. This study aims to
characterize fatigue from long-duration driving, with a special interest in the
use of eye-blink indicators. A total of 12 male commercial drivers participated
in this study, in which they had to drive for about two sessions of 2.5 hours
of actual driving tasks on a highway, separated by 15 minutes of rest. Subjective response to fatigue and sleepiness
was obtained by employing Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS), while several
eye-blink parameters (blink duration, blink frequency, percent of eye closure,
and micro-sleep) were analyzed offline via a continuous recording of the
subject’s facial characteristics throughout the driving task. The findings of
this study demonstrated that driving for roughly 2.5 hours did not result in
undue fatigue. However, an additional driving task of the same duration yielded
excessive fatigue, despite the 15-minute rest period given between the two
trips. All eye-blink parameters were
closely correlated with subjective measures, although more consistent changes
were shown by the blink duration. It was concluded here that prolonged driving,
as part of a professional job, is closely associated with undue fatigue that
represents a road safety risk factor.
Eye-blink parameters; Fatigue; Karolinska sleepiness scale; Prolonged driving; Sleepiness
Road traffic safety has been a very important issue in Indonesia. A report by the Indonesian National Bureau of Statistics (2018) shows a consistently increasing trend of road accidents. A total of more than 109,000 accidents was reported in 2018, an increase of roughly 85% compared to the data in 2008. About 20,000 people were killed in traffic accidents in 2008, while the number was nearly 29,500 in 2018. The latter demonstrates roughly three to four people were killed in road traffic accidents per hour, a statistic that cannot be taken lightly. Direct financial (material) loss in 2018 was estimated to be more than Rp 213 billion (~USD 15 million), not to mention other substantial economic and social loss among those who are affected by the accidents.
While research addressing motor vehicle crashes has been relatively scarce, authorities (such as the Indonesian National Transportation Safety Committee) have pointed out that fatigue and sleepiness are the key contributing factors. These factors have been indicated in most road crashes investigations involving commercial buses and trucks. Though the number is not exactly known, fatigue has also been reported in a good percentage of crashes involving minivans used as shuttle services between major cities. Such services are a very common mode of public transportation in addition to railway.
Among several relevant human-factors
aspects, fatigue has been frequently cited to play a major role in traffic
accidents. The relationship between fatigue and safety have also been addressed
in the literature (Williamson et al., 2011).
Three major factors are closely associated with the development of fatigue,
including time of day, time awake, and task-related factors (Williamson et al., 2011), while
demographic aspects as contributing factors have also been suggested in the
literature (Di-Milia et al., 2011). Fatigue is believed to be a complex
phenomenon (Caldwell et al., 2019),
and no widely accepted definition is currently available. Williamson et
al. (2011) define fatigue as a need for recuperative rest, with
factors including time on task, sleep homeostasis, and time of day, believed to
play a major role to fatigue development. Di-Milia et
al. (2011) suggests an array of potential endogenous as well as
exogenous variables that may be linked to the development of fatigue.
As described previously, fatigue (and
sleepiness) is likely to be prevalent among shuttle service drivers. It is not
uncommon for drivers of these shuttle to operate the vehicle for four trips in
a day, which accumulates to 16 hours of driving (with approximately 30 - 60
minutes of rest between trips). Consequently, the drivers experience lack of
sleep that also affecting its quality. Most of them have to operate long
commute where rest periods are short and uncertain. Self-reported sleepiness
often tends to be bias (Sallinen et al.,
2020), which put the driver and passengers at higher risk of road
crashes. The aim of this study is to
investigate the level of fatigue and sleepiness among shuttle service drivers.
It was hypothesized that the driving job would result in moderate to high level
of fatigue; a phenomenon that the company might not be aware of. The findings of this study could be used as a
strategy to mitigate fatigue as a road safety risk factor.
2.1. Participants
Twelve male drivers aged 30-40
participated in the experiment (mean age 35.6 ± 3.58 years old). They were
compensated for their participation in the experiment. They were experienced
drivers with legitimate driving license for at least 3 years (mean 17.33 ± 3.67
years) and had at least 3 years of driving experience (mean 18.08 ± 4.18
years), with mileage driven more than 5,000 km in the past year (means 7,220 ±
1,416 km). The participants were recruited and selected rather purposively from
several shuttle service companies in Bandung, Indonesia. The age bracket
selected represented a large percentage of commercial (shuttle service) driver
population with adequate year of driving experience. All of them were morning
type people, had normal vision or corrected-to-normal vision (using contact
lenses or glasses), and had no health problems. In addition, all participants
were used to consume drinks containing caffeine, both tea and coffee. A large
portion of the participants (83.3%) were active smokers and none of the
participants consumed alcohol and/or illegal drugs.
A night before the experiment,
participants reported that they had a sufficient amount of sleep (7-8 hours) (mean
7.52 ± 0.54 hours). Prior to conducting the experiment, they filled out a
profile data questionnaire, signed a data confidentiality agreement, and signed
a consent form. Ethical approval for the research protocol was granted by the
Institutional Ethics Review Board prior to data collection.
Before
the experiment began, participants were prohibited from consuming certain foods
or drinks for a certain period of time, other than those permitted by the
researchers. During the experiment, participants were prohibited from listening
to the radio or communicating intensively with the researchers. Participants
were not allowed to wear sunglasses or dark-coloured glasses because they could
interfere with the eye-blink data collection. Based on the pre-driving
self-report, all participants had confirmed to be physically and mentally
healthy, did not consume alcohol within 24 hours before the experiment, and did
not consume caffeine within 4 hours prior to the experiment.
2.2.
Experimental Procedure
Each participant drove a
car from the city of A to the city of B (first session) and back to A (second
session) with a total duration of roughly six hours. Each driver started the task at 8 a.m.; the
morning shift was chosen to represent common, light to moderate workload (as
opposed to a greater workload while driving during night-time). After arriving in B, participants received a
15-minute break before leaving for A. The selection of the route was based on
the traveling duration (more than 150 minutes) according to Falou et al. (2003), monotonous road
conditions (toll roads), as well as low and high traffic densities which
represent low and high workloads (May and Baldwin,
2009; Gimeno, Cerezuela, and Montanes, 2006). Before driving,
participants filled out a questionnaire regarding sleep conditions and other
conditions related to drowsiness before driving. This was done to ensure they
were ready for the experiment, and no prior adverse conditions (such as lack of
sleep) interfered with the results. Participants also reported their subjective
assessment of sleepiness using Karolinska Sleepiness Scale/KSS (Akerstedt et al.,
2014). The KSS was used to capture the
participant’s subjective experience of sleepiness (Kaida
et al., 2006). These
initial data were used as a baseline (the initial conditions before driving).
For each trip, a camera was mounted on
the dashboard facing the driver for the purpose of recording the participant’s
face during the experiment. Off-line, these recordings were analyzed in order
to obtain eye-blink parameters, including frequency, duration, percentage of
eye closure (PERCLOS), and microsleep. Blink duration and frequency are good
indicators for measuring fatigue (Benedetto et
al., 2011; Schleicher et al., 2008). Eye blink data
(including blink frequency and duration) are related to activity, workload, and
fatigue caused by work (Tsai et al., 2007).
Van Orden et al. (2001) stated that
the blink frequency and duration are highly correlated with work and changes in
blink behavior, which are associated with increased levels of fatigue while
doing work. The blink duration was calculated when the eyelid closes until it
opens back completely. The blink frequency was obtained from the number of
blinks in units of time (minutes). These parameters were derived from 1-minute
windows of recording, calculated every 20 minutes throughout the driving
duration. Similarly, scores of KSS were also reported every 20 minutes, in
addition to collecting the scores at the beginning and end of a driving
session.
2.3.
Data Analysis
A descriptive and an
inferential data analysis were done. In this study, non-parametric statistical
test was used because the amount of data processed was less than 30 and did not
meet normal assumptions (Walpole et al.,
2012). This experiment was
designed as repeated measures, with ocular indicator and subjective ratings of
sleepiness as the dependent variables. The interest was to determine changes in
all dependent variables as a function of driving duration. The statistical
analysis was conducted using SPSS software, with a p < 0.05 indicating
significance.
In this study, Wilcoxon
Signed-Rank was used to determine the differences between conditions of the
first session and the second session with 15 minutes of rest. Friedman test was
used to find out the differences between data collection times for each fatigue
indicator value. Finally, the Mann Whitney U test was used to identify which
post-hoc traffic density conditions give a difference in the fatigue
indicators. Correlations (Spearman’s
rho) were conducted to determine the associations among variables (Walpole et al., 2012).
3.1. Experimental
Result
3.1.1. Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS)
Subjective response data demonstrated a
fair increase in sleepiness for the first trip (p < 0.05), with peak score
(KSS = 6) observed toward the end of the first trip (Figure 1). There was a
decrement in the KSS score at the end of the trip. A similar fatigue profile was found with
respect to the second trip (p <0.05). As opposed to the first trip, however, fatigue
was substantially elevated. It is noteworthy that, for the first trip, a KSS of
5 was reported after approximately driving for nearly 2 hours. In contrast, the same KSS for the second trip
was reported before 60 minutes of driving. The baseline values between the two
conditions were significantly different (p < 0.05).
Figure
1 Changes in KSS scores throughout the first (a) and
second trip (b)
3.1.2. Ocular parameters
The first trip of the driving task was
typically characterized by an increase in all ocular parameters (p <
0.05). Except for PERCLOS, all the other
three parameters demonstrated peak values around 60 to 100 minutes, and
subsequent significant (p < 0.05) reductions in eye-blink parameters toward
the end of the task, resulting in somewhat inverted U-shaped curves (Figure 2).
On the other hand, the second trip described in rather different patterns. The inverted U-shaped curves only applied to
blink frequency and microsleep, while the blink duration and PERCLOS tended to
consistently increase over time. Eye-blink duration seemed to be a parameter
that could distinguish fatigue between the two trips. It is worthwhile to
mention that the two trips were also discriminated by the initial (baseline)
values, and when the parameter reached a certain value. Greater baseline values
for the second trip were observed for the blink frequency and blink duration,
whereas the opposite was true for the PERCLOS and microsleep. With respect to
peak values, the blink frequency data showed maximal values at around 60- and
40-minute of driving times for the first and second trips, respectively. For
this peak value, no differences were found for PERCLOS and microsleep. Note
that substantial differences between the two trips were observed for blink
duration, in which the end of the second trip was characterized by markedly
greater duration (p < 0.05).
Figure 2 Changes in ocular parameters as a function of
driving duration. The figures on the left are for the first trip, while the
ones on the right are for the second trip.
3.2. Discussion
This
research was highly motivated by the fact that road crashes involving
commercial drivers are fairly common in Indonesia. Current law only regulates
the hours of service; but enforcement of such law is doubtful, even among
public transportation companies. Though no in-depth studies addressing this
concern are available, it is not uncommon for a commercial driver to work for
14-16 hours during their work shift.
Informal observations among drivers operating shuttle services often
demonstrate fatigue-related behavior, such as driving slowly in the right
(passing) lane, speeding while at lower gears, singing incoherently, rolling
down the window, rubbing the faces, or consuming candies frequently. All these
symptoms clearly show behavioral responses to counter fatigue and sleepiness.
Such phenomena certainly pose a safety risk, but this issue has been very
rarely quantified and investigated. Several research findings pertain to
fatigue and sleepiness have been reported in Indonesia (Zuraida
and Abbas, 2020; Zuraida, Iridiastadi, and Sutalaksana, 2017; Puspasari et al.,
2017, 2015; Muslim et al., 2015). These investigations,
however, were conducted by utilizing driving simulators, with findings that
could not directly be generalized within the field contexts. In contrast, this present
study aimed at examining sleepiness as a result of prolonged driving duration
experienced by commercial drivers.
For the driving activities investigated
in this study, it was found that driving for about 2.5 hours clearly induced a
moderate level of sleepiness, and an additional trip of the same duration
resulted in excessive level of sleepiness.
Moreover, following a driving duration of 2.5 hours with a rest period
of 15 minutes was indeed not adequate.
Lastly, fatigue and sleepiness were also characterized by relatively
consistent changes in ocular indicators, particularly blink duration, and
PERCLOS. These measures closely correlated to subjective reports of sleepiness.
It should be noted that these findings were limited to only one task
characteristic (task duration) and the fact that the drivers’ age and gender
were not examined, which could restrict generalization of the results.
3.2.1. Fatigue and time on task
Previous
studies generally reported fatigue patterns that vary. It can be influenced by
number of factors, including the indicators used, whether the experiment was
conducted as a field study or a lab experiment, or the type of factors
manipulated (e.g. sleep durations prior to the experiment). The work of Zuraida et al. (2019), for instance, shows
the progression of sleepiness that tended to be linear. In contrast, Puspasari et al. (2019) found exponential
patterns, particularly for drivers who did not receive enough sleep the night
before. Mixed patterns of fatigue were
also reported by Ingre et al. (2006).
Participants who received an adequate amount of sleep at night were
characterized by a fairly gradual increase of fatigue, whereas those who had to
stay awake tended to be characterized by a sudden (exponential) increase of
fatigue. This study further noted the
presence of individual differences that should be taken into consideration when
analyzing driving fatigue.
Patterns
of fatigue have also been reported in studies examining the effects of fatigue
during acute sleep deprivation (lasting 24 hours or more). Petrilli et al.
(2005) examined subjects who undertook 24 hours of sustained
wakefulness. Fatigue was evaluated by assessing changes in alertness as
measured by psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) and visual analogue scale (VAS).
Alertness tended to improve in the first couple of hours, but then generally
(and gradually) worsened in the following hours. Substantial decrements in
fatigue were found after 10 hours, and especially after 14 hours of
wakefulness. Their study demonstrated that fatigue followed non-linear
patterns, and inconsistent changes could be expected within the first few hours
of wakefulness. In their study, Alvaro et al.
(2016) found infrequent episodes of prolonged eyelid closure during the
first 14 hours of sleep deprivation. A substantial increase of the measures was
found after 17 hours of awake time. All these studies indicate that
characterizing fatigue is rather difficult and strongly dependent upon the
context being studied.
One of the major issues
pertaining to fatigue resulted from long duration driving is how long a driver
is allowed to drive continuously, before fatigue sets in and starts to impair
driving performance. Furthermore, the
amount of rest period following a driving task is often the subject of
interest, considered as an intervention strategy to reduce the effects of
fatigue. This study found that driving for about 2.5 hours resulted in moderate
level of fatigue, and that a 15-minute rest period did not return fatigue
measures to their baseline values. Previous studies have shown that 2 to 3
hours of driving (following a restful night sleep) generally do not yield
excessive fatigue and sleepiness (Zuraida et al.,
2019; Puspasari et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018; Di-Stasi et
al., 2012; Craig et al., 2011). However, discrepancies do
exist due to differences in experimental settings (field vs. simulator) and
also due to inter-individual differences (Ingre et
al., 2006). The majority of
investigations addressing fatigue from driving have been done by employing a
simulator, which could potentially induce monotony that results in a greater
feeling of fatigue. This issue, however, has not been widely and specifically
addressed in previous investigations.
3.2.2. Degrees of fatigue
Findings
from this and previous studies indicate that patterns of fatigue and sleepiness
may not be consistent across experimental conditions. Except using subjective
response such as KSS, it is probably difficult to categorize fatigue based on
ocular measures. It is not clear, for
example, what a 10% vs. 30% increase in blink durations really implies, or how
we might draw a line between drowsy and awake/alert conditions using ocular
measures. Except in a few studies (Dreißig et al., 2020; Puspasari et
al., 2019), little has
been done with respect to classifying the degrees of fatigue as a function of
driving tasks.
The
work of Puspasari et al. (2019) suggested
the cut-off value of the blink duration and PERCLOS between alert vs. low-level
fatigue vs. heavy fatigue conditions. However, it is also stated in the report
that the practical implication of the cut-off value should be investigated
further due to the small sample size of the study. An attempt to classify the
driver’s drowsiness state was also made by Dreißig et al. (2020), by developing a feature
selection method based on the k-Nearest Neighbor algorithm. They utilized a
large dataset of eye blink and head movements behaviours extracted from driving
simulation experiments to train the machine learning model. Nonetheless, a
validation of real-world data has not been conducted to confirm the model’s
robustness.
Schleicher et al. (2008) reviewed a variety
of indicators and parameters that can be employed in assessing fatigue, ranging
from the ‘gold standard’ electroencephalography (EEG) to measures of
performance and subjective responses such as KSS. However, the degrees of
fatigue could only be represented by a handful of measures, including
self-rated responses, observer-rated facial behaviour, and microsleeps. An
increase in only blink frequency could mean the presence of a low level of
fatigue, while simultaneous changes in this measure and blink duration could be
categorized as a severe level of fatigue. The subjective response is
susceptible to biases, but it is considered as the best choice with respect to
the ability in distinguishing the degrees of fatigue. Their study further noted
blink duration (particularly microsleep) as a measure that correlates well with
subjective responses. The work of Ingre et al.
(2006) demonstrated similar findings, in that greater KSS scores were
closely related to longer blink duration and poorer driving performance.
Using
a simulator, Wang et al. (2018) investigated
the effects of driving durations on oculomotor, driving performance, and
subjective responses. Compared to baseline data, blink duration increased by
about 23% after 2 hours of driving, and prolonged driving to 3 and 4 hours
resulted in an increase of blink duration of nearly 115% and 190%,
respectively. For each respected driving duration, their experiment showed
blink frequency increases for about 3%, 16%, and 25%. Note that the peak level of subjective rating
of sleepiness only reached about the mid-level of the Stanford Sleepiness Scale
(SSS).
It
should be noted, however, that levels of fatigue should not be evaluated solely
on the basis of aggregate values, due to the presence of individual differences
(Ingre et al., 2006). Thus,
comparisons of absolute data across different studies may not be adequate.
During a 2-hour simulated driving task, data from Ingre
et al. (2006) demonstrated that excessive fatigue (resulting from
sleep deprived condition) could result in an increase of blink duration that
was twice as large as the baseline data.
In a similar experiment, Puspasari et al.
(2019) showed undue fatigue that was characterized by more than a five-
to eight-fold increase in three oculomotor parameters (blink duration, PERCLOS,
and microsleep).
This study clearly demonstrated that driving for about 2.5 hours
only resulted in a moderate level of fatigue, but the 15 minutes of rest
between driving tasks was not adequate. This result can be used by relevant
stakeholders (e.g., the Ministry of Transportation or toll way operator) as a
basis in determining work-rest schedules for car drivers. Relevant government
regulations pertaining to this issue are available and can, thus, be modified
based on the results of this (and other similar) studies.
3.2.3. Ocular indicators
This
investigation demonstrated that all eyelid measurement parameters used could
indeed be used for the purpose of fatigue evaluation during a prolonged driving
task. Except for the final phase of the
driving segments, these parameters consistently changed as a function of
driving duration. Eyeblink duration and PERCLOS were found to be highly
correlated with a subjective report of sleepiness (KSS), while the other two parameters
(blink frequency and microsleep) were moderately correlated with KSS. Previous
research generally agrees that different parameters of the eyeblink indicator
could be used as an objective, non-invasive approach in the assessments of
fatigue and drowsiness (Cori et al., 2019).
Such an approach takes advantage over other subjective measures; it can be done
continuously (via the use of a camera) without interfering with the driving
task. The findings of this study are also in agreement with what Cori et al. (2019) have stated, that the
use of blink duration and PERCLOS are two of the most robust parameters. At the
moment, it is suggested that further investigations be carried out that study
the performance of eyeblink indicators in various driving contexts found in the
field. The availability of
high-definition cameras and analysis software have allowed for real-time
assessment of driver fatigue.
Furthermore,
fatigue has often been dealt with the availability of technology that allows
real-time interventions. The technology
will provide a warning system (visual, auditory, or haptic) based on inputs in
a form of ocular measures. A contribution of this study is that there are
certain ocular parameters and criterion that can be employed with the use of
technology as a means to minimize fatigue risks. It should be emphasized that blink duration
was found to be a parameter with greatest association with subjective reports
of sleepiness. Therefore, any fatigue detection technology that works based on
blink duration profiles should have a certain degree of validity. The high
correlation also means that different criteria (such as the onset of
sleepiness) can be further established by following KSS classifications
commonly reported in the literature.
It can be concluded here that
driving for about 2.5 hours only induced a fairly moderate level of fatigue and
sleepiness. However, the same amount of
additional driving time was found to result in undue fatigue, despite the 15
minutes break provided following the first trip. The majority of eye-blink parameters
also changed as a function of driving time, but blink duration was the only
parameter that consistently increased throughout the driving task. This study
suggests that shuttle companies should give more thoughtful attention to
fatigue issues among their drivers and provide all the necessary precautions to
mitigate fatigue risk. The findings of this study can also be used as a basis
to evaluate driver rostering. It should be noted that further investigations
are warranted, particularly the ones that examine the effects of rest periods between
trips. Furthermore, it is certainly an interesting research issue if the onset
of fatigue can be confidently determined, or if fatigue has become excessive
that it interferes with driving performance. The development of drowsiness
classification model is indeed an important requirement for the advancement of
driver’s fatigue detection technology.
Akerstedt, T., Anund, A., Axelsson, J., Kecklund, G., 2014.
Subjective Sleepiness Is a Sensitive Indicator of Insufficient Sleep and Impaired
Waking Function. Journal of Sleep
Research, Volume 23(3), pp. 240–252 (not found in the text)
Alvaro, P.K., Jackson, M.L., Berlowitz, D.J., Swann, P., Howard,
M.E., 2016. Prolonged Eyelid Closure Episodes during Sleep Deprivation in
Professional Drivers. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Volume 12(8),
pp. 1099–1103
Benedetto, S., Pedrotti, M., Minin, L., Baccino, T., Re, A.,
Montanari, R., 2011. Driver Workload and Eye Blink Duration. Transportation
Research Part F, Volume 14, pp. 199–208
Caldwell, J.A., Caldwell, J.L., Thompson, L.A., Lieberman, H.R.,
2019. Fatigue and Its Management in The Workplace. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, Volume 96, pp. 272–289
Cori, J. M., Anderson, C., Soleimanloo, S.S., Jackson, M.L.,
Howard, M.E., 2019. Narrative Review: Do Spontaneous Eye Blink Parameters
Provide a Useful Assessment of State Drowsiness? Sleep Medicine Reviews,
Volume 45, pp. 95–104
Craig, A., Tran, Y., Wijesuriya, N., Nguyen, H., 2012. Regional Brain
Wave Activity Changes Associated with Fatigue. Psychophysiology, Volume
49(4), pp. 574–582
Di-Milia, L., Smolensky, M.H., Costa, G., Howarth, H.D., Ohayon,
M.M., Philip, P., 2011. Demographic Factors, Fatigue, and Driving Accidents: An
Examination of The Published Literature. Accident Analysis and Prevention,
Volume 43(2), pp. 516–532
Di-Stasi, L.L., Renner, R., Catena, A., Canas, J.J., Velichkovsky,
B.M., Pannasch, S., 2012. Towards A Driver Fatigue Test Based on The Saccadic
Main Sequence: A Partial Validation by Subjective Report Data. Transportation
Research Part C, Volume 21, pp. 122–133
M., Baccour, M. H., Schack, T., Kasneci, E., 2020. Driver
Drowsiness Classification Based on Eye Blink and Head Movement Features Using
the k-NN Algorithm. In 2020 IEEE Symposium Series on Computational
Intelligence (SSCI), pp. 889-896
Falou, W.E., Duchene, J., Grabisch, M., Hewson, D., Langeron, Y.,
Lino, F, 2003. Evaluation of Driver Discomfort During Long-Duration Car Driving.
Applied Ergonomics, Volume 34(3), pp. 249–255
Gimeno, P.T., Cerezuela, G.P., Montanes, M.C., 2006. On The Concept
and Measurement of Driver Drowsiness, Fatigue, and Inattention: Implications
for Countermeasures. International Journal of Vehicle Design, Volume 42,
pp. 67–85
Ingre, M., Akerstedt, T., Peters, B., Anund, A., Kecklund, G.,
2006. Subjective Sleepiness, Simulated Driving Performance and Blink Duration:
Examining Individual Differences. Journal of Sleep Research, Volume 15,
pp. 47–53
Indonesian National Bureau of Statistics, 2018. Jumlah Kecelakaan,
Korban Mati, Luka Berat, Luka Ringan, dan Kerugian Materi 2016–2018. http:// https://www.bps.go.id/indicator/17/513/2/jumlah-kecelakaan-korban-mati-luka-berat-luka-ringan-dan-kerugian-materi.html
Kaida, K., Takahasi, M., Akerstedt, T., Nakata, A., Otsuka, Y.,
Haratani, T., Fukasawa, K., 2006. Validation of the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale
Against Performance and EEG Variables. Clinical Neurophysiology, Volume
117 (7), pp. 1574–1581
May, J., Baldwin, C.L., 2009. Driver fatigue: The importance of
identifying causal factors of fatigue when considering detection and
countermeasure technologies. Transportation Research Part F, Volume 12, pp.
218–224
Muslim, E., Moch, B.N., Puspasari, M.A., Siregar, R.A., 2015. Fatigue
Measurement of Driving Activity on Male Motorcycle Drivers based on Cognitive,
Physiological, and Subjective Approaches. International Journal of
Technology, Volume 6(6), 976–982
Petrilli, R.M., Jay, S.M., Dawson, D., Lamond, N., 2005. The Impact
of Sustained Wakefulness and Time-of-Day on OSPAT Performance. Industrial
Health, Volume 43 (1), pp. 186–192
Puspasari, M.A., Iridiastadi, H., Sutalaksana, I.Z., Sjafruddin,
A., 2019. Ocular Indicators as Fatigue Detection Instruments for Indonesian
Drivers. Industrial Engineering and Management Systems, Volume 18(4),
pp. 748–760
Puspasari, M.A., Iridiastadi, H., Sutalaksana, I.Z., Sjafruddin,
A., 2018. Fatigue Classification of Ocular Indicators Using Support Vector
Machine. Proceedings of International Conference on Intelligent Informatics
and Biomedical Sciences (ICIIBMS), Volume 3, pp. 66–69
Puspasari, M.A., Iridiastadi, H.I., Sutalaksana, I.Z., dan
Sjafruddin, A., 2017. Effect of driving duration towards EEG fluctuations. International
Journal of Technology, 6, 1089–1096
Puspasari, M.A., Muslim, E., Moch, B.N., Aristides, A., 2015.
Fatigue Measurement in Car Driving Activity using Physiological, Cognitive, and
Subjective Approaches, International Journal of Technology, Volume 6(6),
pp. 971–975
Sallinen, M., Pylkkonen, M., Puttonen, S., Sihvola, M., Akerstedt,
T., 2020. Are Long-Haul Truck Drivers Unusually Alert? A Comparison with
Long-Haul Airline Pilots. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Volume
137, pp. 105442
Schleicher, R., Galley, N., Briest, S., Galley, L., 2008. Blinks
and Saccades as Indicators of Fatigue in Sleepiness Warnings: Looking Tired?. Ergonomics,
Volume 51, pp 982–1010
Tsai, Y.F, Viirre, E., Strychacz, C., Chase, B., Jung, T.P, 2007.
Task Performance and Eye Activity: Predicting Behavior Relating to Cognitive
Workload. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, Volume 78, pp. 176–185
Van Orden, K.F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., Jung, T.P., 2001. Eye Activity
Correlates of Workload During A Visuospatial Memory Task. Human factors: The
Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Volume 43(1), pp.
111–121
Walpole, R.E., Myers, R.H., Myers, S.L., Ye. K., 2012. Probability and Statistics for Engineers and Scientists. Boston: Prentice Hall
Wang, F., Zhang, X., Fu, R., Sun, G., 2018. EEG Characteristic
Analysis of Coach Bus Drivers Based on Brain Conectivity as Revealed via A
Graph Theoritical Network. The Royal Society of Chemistry, Volume 8, pp.
29745–29755
Williamson, A., Lombardi, D.A., Folkard, S., Stutts, J., Courtney,
T.K., Connor, J.L., 2011. The Link Between Fatigue and Safety. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, Volume 43(2), pp. 498–515
Zuraida, R., Iridiastadi, H., Sutalaksana, I.Z., 2017. Indonesian
Drivers’ Characteristics Associated with Road Accidents. International
Journal of Technology, Volume 8(2), pp. 311–319
Zuraida, R., Iridiastadi, H., Sutalaksana, I., Suprijanto, 2019. An
Analysis of EEG Changes during Prolonged Simulated Driving for the Assessment
of Driver Fatigue. ITB Journal of Engineering Science, Volume 51, pp. 290–302
Zuraida, R., Abbas, B.S., 2020. The Factors Influencing Fatigue
Related to the Accident of Intercity Bus Drivers in Indonesia. International
Journal of Technology. Volume 11(2), pp. 342–352